Your favorite athlete closes in for avictorious win. The crowd holds its breath, and, at the crucial moment, shemisses the shot. That competitor just experienced the phenomenon known as “choking,”where despite months, even years, of practice, a person fails right when itmatters most. Choking is common in sports, where performance often occurs underintense pressure and depends on key moments. And yet, performance anxiety alsohaunts public speakers, contestants in spelling bees, and event world-famousmusicians. Most people intuitively blame it on their nerves, but why does beingnervous undermine expert performance? There are two sets of theories, whichboth say that primarily, choking under pressure boils down to focus. First,there are the distraction theories. These suggests that performance sufferswhen the mind is preoccupied with worries, doubts, or fears, instead offocusing its attention on performing the task at hand, when relevant and irrelevantthoughts compete for the same attention, something has to give. The brain canonly process so much information at once. Task that challenge working memory,the mental ”scratch pad” we use to temporarily store phone numbers and grocerylists, are especially vulnerable to pressure. In a 2004 study, a group ofuniversity students were asked to perform math problems, some easy, others morecomplex and memory-intensive. Half the students completed both problem typeswith nothing at stake, while the others completed them when calm and underpressure. While every did well on the easy problems, those who were stressedperformed worse on the more difficult, memory-intensive tasks. Explicitmonitoring theories make up the second group of explanations for choking underpressure. They are concerned with how pressure can cause people to overanalyzethe task at hand. Here, the logic goes that once a skill becomes automatic,thinking about its precise mechanics interferes with your ability to do it.Tasks we do unconsciously seem to be most vulnerable to this kind of choking. Astudy on competitive golfers compared their performance when instruct to simplyfocus on putting as accurately as possible, versus when they were primed to beacutely aware of the mechanics of their putting stroke. Golfers usually performthis action subconsciously, so those who suddenly tuned in to the precisedetails of their own moves also became worse at making accurate shots. Chokingmay not be inevitable for everyone though. Research suggests that some are moresusceptible than others, especially those who are self-conscious, anxious, andafraid of being judged negatively by others. So, how can we avoid choking whenit really counts? First, it helps to practice under stressful conditions. In astudy on expert dart players, researchers found that those who hadn’t practicedunder stress, performed worse when anxious, compared to those who had becomeaccustomed to pressure. Secondly, many performers extol the virtues of apre-performance routine, whether it’s taking a few deep breaths, repeating acue word, or doing a rhythmic sequence of movements. Studies on golfing,bowling, and water polo find that short rituals can lead to more consistent,and accurate performance under pressure. And thirdly, researchers have shownthat having an external focus on the ultimate goal works better than aninternal focus, where someone is tuned into the mechanics of what they’re doing.A study of experienced golfers revealed that those who hit chip shots whilefocused on the flight of the ball performed sidnificantly better than those whofocused on the motion of their arms. So, perhaps we can modify that age-oldsaying: practice, under pressure with focus, and with that glorious end goal insight, makes perfact.
你最爱的运动员即将迎来制胜一击,观众们屏住呼吸,但在这重要时刻,她失误了。这位选手刚经历了“反胜为败”现象,纵使进行了经年累月的练习,人们仍会在关键时刻失败。“反胜为败”在运动界是一种普遍现象,因为运动员的表现都在压力下进行,并且会受到比赛节点的影响。这种焦虑也会影响演讲者,拼字游戏中的选手们,以及世界著名的音乐家。绝大多数人凭直觉将其归咎于精神紧张,但为何紧张的情绪会影响专业人士的表现呢?两组理论对此进行了解释。二者都认为压力下的“反胜为败”归根结底是注意力问题。第一种理论是干扰理论。该理论认为当担忧,疑惑和恐惧占据了人们的头脑,让人们无法集中注意力。在手头的任务,其表现就会受到影响。当与任务相关和无关的信息争夺注意力时,其中一方不得不做出让步。大脑能够同时处理的信息量有限。工作记忆是我们头脑中的“速写板”,我们用它短暂存储电话号码和购物清单等。对于高度要求工作记忆的任务来说,压力对其影响尤为重要。在2004年的一项研究中,一组大学生被要求解答数学问题,一些题目简单,另一些则非常困难,且需要大量记忆。一些学生在没有任何顾忌的情况下解答全部题目,另一半学生分别在冷静状态和压力状态下完成题目。尽管所有人在简单问题上表现良好,但对于更困难且需要记忆的题目来说,经受压力的学生表现更差。第二套解释该现象的理论是外部控制理论。这一理论是关于压力如何使人们过度分析当前的任务。这种说法的逻辑在于一旦某种技能成为下意识的熟练技能,思考其精确的原理机制就会妨碍使用该技能。需要下意识完成的任务最容易出现“反胜为败”。一项研究对比了高尔夫选手的表现,一组选手只需要关注准确挥杆,另一组则被要求时刻想着挥杆动作是否符合标准原理。高尔夫选手通常依靠潜意识完成动作,因此在选手突然要调整动作达到标准时,他们反而更难打出准确一击。虽然并非所有人都会经历“反胜为败”,但研究表明,部分人群更容易受到影响,尤其是那些自我意识较强的,容易焦虑的,害怕他人负面评价的人。既然“反胜为败”影响如此之大,我们应该如何避免它的出现呢?首先,在压力环境下练习会有所帮助。在一项关于专业飞镖选手的研究中,研究者发现,从未在压力下练习过的选手,在焦虑时比习惯压力的选手表现更差。第二,许多运动员推崇进行赛前例行动作,例如进行几次深呼吸,重复某个提示词,或者进行有节奏的一系列动作。在高尔夫,保龄球和水球方面的研究发现,进行简短的仪式性动作可以使选手在压力下的表现更为正常和准确。第三,研究者表明,将注意力集中在实现最终目标上,比关注在内部调整自己符合规范动作原理更加有效。一项高尔夫研究表明,在切削击球的经验丰富的选手中,那些关注球的飞行路线的选手比关注自己手臂运动的选手表现好得多。因此我们也许可以修改那句古老谚语,“练习,在压力下,保持专注,锁定目标的练习,才会熟能生巧”。
网友评论